Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Online first
    • Online first
  • Current issue
    • Current issue
  • Archive
    • Archive
  • Submit a paper
    • Online submission site
    • Information for authors
  • About the journal
    • About the journal
    • Editorial board
    • Information for authors
    • FAQs
    • Thank you to our reviewers
      • Thank you to our reviewers
    • American Federation for Medical Research
  • Help
    • Contact us
    • Feedback form
    • Reprints
    • Permissions
    • Advertising
  • BMJ Journals

User menu

  • Login

Search

  • Advanced search
  • BMJ Journals
  • Login
  • Facebook
  • Twitter
JIM

Advanced Search

  • Online first
    • Online first
  • Current issue
    • Current issue
  • Archive
    • Archive
  • Submit a paper
    • Online submission site
    • Information for authors
  • About the journal
    • About the journal
    • Editorial board
    • Information for authors
    • FAQs
    • Thank you to our reviewers
    • American Federation for Medical Research
  • Help
    • Contact us
    • Feedback form
    • Reprints
    • Permissions
    • Advertising

Predictors of misperceptions, risk perceptions, and personal risk perceptions about COVID-19 by country, education and income

Tanzim Bhuiya, Richard Klares III, Madellena A Conte, Joseph Steven Cervia
DOI: 10.1136/jim-2021-001835 Published 25 November 2021
Tanzim Bhuiya
Donald and Barbara Zucker School of Medicine at Hofstra/Northwell, Manhasset, New York, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Tanzim Bhuiya
Richard Klares III
Donald and Barbara Zucker School of Medicine at Hofstra/Northwell, Manhasset, New York, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Richard Klares III
Madellena A Conte
Donald and Barbara Zucker School of Medicine at Hofstra/Northwell, Manhasset, New York, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Madellena A Conte
Joseph Steven Cervia
Donald and Barbara Zucker School of Medicine at Hofstra/Northwell, Manhasset, New York, USA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • ORCID record for Joseph Steven Cervia
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • eLetters
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Government interventions, such as mandating the use of masks and social distancing, play crucial roles in controlling the spread of pandemic infection. Adherence depends on public perceptions about pandemic risk. The goal was to explore the roles of education, income, and country on misperceptions, risk perceptions and personal risk perceptions about COVID-19. Data were extracted from 3 preregistered surveys. Binary logistic regressions were conducted to investigate the roles country, education, and income had on outcome variables. Across the USA, Canada, and UK, individuals in the highest income quartile were significantly less likely to hold misperceptions (OR=0.61, 95% CI 0.45 to 0.83) and to perceive personal risk (OR=0.38, 95% CI 0.20 to 0.75) regarding COVID-19 compared with individuals in the lowest income quartile. When comparing these income quartiles in the USA, the difference in perceived risk was heightened (OR=0.21, 95% CI 0.07 to 0.57). Citizens of the UK were more likely to have risk perceptions compared with citizens of the USA (OR=1.50, 95% CI 1.20 to 1.88). Citizens of Canada were less likely to perceive personal risk compared with US citizens (OR=0.40, 95% CI 0.23 to 0.69). Proper risk perception and understanding of COVID-19 are necessary for adherence to public health initiatives. The lowest income quartile was shown to have more misperceptions and personal risk perceptions across all 3 countries, highlighting the disproportionate impact of COVID-19 in this group. Our findings support the importance of education and income in affecting health perceptions and outcomes. Further research is needed to explore interventions to minimize misperceptions, accurately shape risk perception, and effectively communicate science.

Significance of this study

What is already known about this subject?

  • It is important for the public to adhere to government recommendations in order to properly control the spread of COVID-19.

  • The main considerations the public takes before accepting a protective action recommendation from the government include susceptibility to the disease, severity of the epidemic, effectiveness of protective actions and the cost of taking protective actions.

  • As a result, the public’s misperceptions, overall risk perceptions, and personal risk perceptions play a substantial role in public health initiative adherence and overall spread of disease.

What are the new findings?

  • Individuals of lower socioeconomic or educational status are more likely to have misperceptions regarding COVID-19, indicating difficulty differentiating between falsehoods and reality regarding the disease.

  • Risk perception about COVID-19 differed between countries, and the personal risk perception citizens felt was influenced by both country and income.

  • Lower income was associated with greater personal risk perception across all 3 countries (USA, UK, and Canada).

How might these results change the focus of research or clinical practice?

  • The present work indicates the statistically significant findings that education and income play a role in predicting misperceptions; country of origin predicts risk perception; and country of origin and income predict personal risk perception. Our findings support the importance of education and income in affecting health perceptions and outcomes. Further research is needed to explore interventions to minimize misperceptions, accurately shape risk perception, and effectively communicate science.

Introduction

In December 2019, cases of pneumonia of unknown cause emerged in Wuhan city, Hubei province of China. The WHO began to investigate these cases on January 1, 2020.1 By January 9, it was determined that hospitalized patients were infected with SARS-CoV-2.2 3 Over the next month, the first reported death due to SARS-CoV-2 infection was reported in China (January 11) and cases began to appear in Thailand (January 13), Japan (January 16), USA (January 21), France (January 24), Canada (January 26), and the UK (January 31).1 4–6 By February 11, the WHO announced the disease resulting from infection with SARS-CoV-2 would be called COVID-19.1–3 On March 11, COVID-19 was declared a pandemic by WHO and by April 4 there were over 1 million cases of COVID-19 worldwide.1

To take a closer look at the impact of COVID-19, one can look to the USA, Canada, and the UK, where this became clear in the months of March and April. In the USA, California had issued state-wide stay at home orders, the University of Minnesota began to test hydroxychloroquine as therapy for COVID-19, and the Coronavirus Aid, Relief, and Economic Security Act was signed into law by the US government.4 The US–Canada border closed in late March (March 21) and in early April (April 13) nearly 5.4 million Canadians were receiving emergency aid from the federal government.5 The end of March (March 23) also brought a UK-wide lockdown, although a £600 million (US$773 million) infection control package was not seen until early May (May 13).7

In addition to the response from governments to the COVID-19 pandemic, there has been widespread response from citizens, and misinformation being spread on social media platforms. Several conspiracy theories surrounding COVID-19 have contributed to misperceptions about the virus. Such conspiracies include the following: COVID-19 was engineered in a Wuhan lab, COVID-19 is no worse than the influenza, masks are unnecessary, and that wealthy elites are behind the virus to make more money.8 This leaves the world in a state where officials and scientists must work to fight COVID-19, and to remedy the misperceptions that people hold.

Studies have used mathematical models to show that government interventions play a crucial role in controlling the spread of disease. Other studies have shown that when there is a lack of knowledge and treatment for a virus, behavioral measures are required to control the disease.9 To prevent the further spread of COVID-19, government agencies have to launch successful public health initiatives and implement aggressive interventions, such as social distancing, restricted movements and mandatory mask use. However, it is crucial for the public to participate and adhere to the government’s control measures. This includes bolstering their own knowledge of the virus and taking initiatives to change their health behaviors.

It is important for the public to adhere to government recommendations in order to properly control the spread of COVID-19. The health belief model (HBM) is a behavioral change theory constructed in order to highlight which beliefs should be communicated to a population in order to cause positive health behaviors.10 The HBM highlights the factors involved in the public’s willingness to adjust their health behavior during an epidemic outbreak. Though the correlation between HBM variables and health behavior varies in strength, they can be useful in providing a basic framework of factors that assess health behavior changes. 11 The main considerations the public takes before accepting a protective action recommendation from the government include susceptibility to the disease, severity of the epidemic, effectiveness of protective actions and the cost of taking protective actions.9 As a result, the public’s misperceptions, overall risk perceptions, and personal risk perceptions play a substantial role in public health initiative adherence and overall spread of disease.

Because attitudes about COVID-19 (ie, misperception and risk perception) have tremendous influence over the success of public health measures to curb COVID-19 transmission, it is essential to understand the factors that affect attitudes about COVID-19. Previous work has explored the role of political ideology and cognitive sophistication in explaining attitudes and misperceptions about COVID-19 across countries, such as the USA, UK and Canada.12 However, there is a gap in our understanding of how education and income influence COVID-19 attitudes and misperceptions. Research supports the stark disparities in COVID-19 health outcomes across education and income level and understanding the role of such factors in determining COVID-19 attitudes and misperceptions is essential to both improve the success of public health measures and minimize these disparities.13 This study investigates the roles of education and income in predicting misperceptions, risk perceptions, and personal risk perceptions about COVID-19 among residents of the USA, Canada, and the UK.

Materials and methods

This study used open-source data supplied by Pennycook et al.12 Data were extracted from preregistered survey conducted in all 3 countries by the polling firm Prolific. Parallel quota sampling was conducted for residents of the USA (n=689) and the UK (n=642). Convenience sampling was conducted for residents from Canada (n=644). Three key outcomes were analyzed in this study: (1) misperceptions about COVID-19, (2) risk perceptions about COVID-19, and (3) personal risk perceptions about COVID-19. Binary logistic regressions were conducted in order to investigate the roles country of residence, education and household income had on the survey outcomes.

Survey categories

Misperceptions about COVID-19 were measured through the creation of a large list of falsehoods (k=21) that were measured on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Examples include ‘the seasonal flu is just as dangerous as the coronavirus’. The overall average score was calculated and then dichotomized. If a participant scored a 2 or below, this signified that the participant overall disagreed with the misperceptions presented. A score above 2 implied that the participant had internalized some misperceptions.

Risk perceptions were assessed using 8 direct questions such as ‘the coronavirus poses a major threat to the public’. Responses were measured on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. The overall average score was calculated and then dichotomized. A score of 6 or above implied the participant perceived risk from COVID-19, while a score below 6 signified the participant did not perceive risk from COVID-19.

Personal risk perceptions were assessed using 3 direct questions such as ‘because of my age and/or pre-existing conditions, I am likely to have serious symptoms if I were to contract the coronavirus’. Responses were measured on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. The overall average score was calculated and then dichotomized. A score of 6 or above implied the participant perceived personal risk from COVID-19 while a score below 6 signified the participant did not perceive personal risk from COVID-19.

Demographic variables

The independent variables used in this study were country of residence, education and household income as seen in figure 1. The countries studied included the USA, the UK and Canada. Education was dichotomized into participants with and without a college degree. Household income was divided into quartiles where the lowest quartile included households who made less than $29,999, the third highest quartile included participants who had a household income of $30,000–$59,999, the second highest quartile included participants who had a household income of $60,000–$89,000, and the highest quartile included participants who had a household income greater than $90,000.

Figure 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1

Demographics.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted using SPSS V.27.0 software.14 Three separate binary logistic regression models were performed to predict: misperception (yes/no), risk perception (yes/no) and personal risk perception (yes/no). The following variables were entered into the models as predictors: country (dummy coded with USA as the reference), education status (college degree vs no college degree), and income (dummy coded with lowest quartile as reference). Models were considered significant if their Omnibus Tests of Model Coefficients were less than 0.05. If the model was significant, specific independent variables within the model were considered significant if their p values were less than 0.05.

Results

Misperception of falsehoods

Our model was able to significantly predict individual levels of misperception regarding falsehoods regarding COVID-19 when looking across all 3 countries. Individuals in the lowest quartile of income held more misperceptions about COVID-19 than did individuals in the second highest or highest quartiles of income (OR=0.66 (0.49, 0.88), p=0.005 and OR=0.61 (0.45, 0.83), p=0.001, respectively). Furthermore, individuals without a college degree held more misperceptions than those with a college degree (OR=0.74 (0.61, 0.92), p=0.003). When looking within each country, our model proved significant within each country. However, within Canada and the USA, no individual predictors were significant. Within the UK, individuals within the lowest quartile of income held more misperceptions than individuals in the second highest or highest quartiles of income (OR=0.51 (0.26, 0.99), p=0.047 and OR=0.21 (0.07, 0.57), p=0.002, respectively). Lastly, those without a college degree in the UK held more misperceptions about COVID-19 than did those with a college degree (OR=1.56 (CI)) (figure 2).

Figure 2
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2

Predictors of misperception regarding COVID-19 by country, education, and income.

Risk perception

An individual’s country of residence was the only significant predictor of COVID-19 risk perception when looking across all 3 countries (USA, Canada, and UK). Specifically, residents of the UK were more likely to have COVID-19 risk perceptions compared with residents of the USA (OR=1.50 (1.20–1.88), p<0.001). Within each country, this logistic regression model was an insignificant predictor of COVID-19 risk perception (figure 3).

Figure 3
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3

Predictors of risk perception regarding COVID-19 by country, education, and income.

Personal risk perception

Related to COVID-19 risk perception is an individual’s level of perceived personal risk. Personal risk perception offers a more nuanced view of a citizen’s individualized risk of contracting the disease as opposed to the more generalized risk the country faces. Our model proved significant across all 3 countries, with participants in the lowest quartile of income perceiving more personal risks than those in the third highest or highest quartiles of income (OR=0.54 (0.34, 0.86), p=0.009 and OR=0.38 (0.20, 0.75), p=0.005, respectively). Residents of the USA perceived more personal risks than residents of Canada (OR=0.40 (0.23, 0.69), p=0.001). When analyzed within each country, our model only proved significant within the USA, where individuals in the lowest quartile of income perceived a greater level of personal risk than individuals in the third highest and highest quartiles of income (OR=0.51 (0.26, 0.99), p=0.047 and OR=0.21 (0.07, 0.57), p=0.002, respectively). ORs of all predictor variables from all statistically significant logistic regressions are included in the online supplemental material (figure 4)

Supplementary data

[jim-2021-001835supp001.pdf]
Figure 4
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 4

Predictors of personal risk perception regarding COVID-19 by country, education, and income.

Discussion

This analysis reveals that individuals of lower socioeconomic or educational status are more likely to have misperceptions regarding COVID-19, indicating difficulty differentiating between falsehoods and reality regarding the disease. It seems intuitive that those with less education would have less experience in the realm of medicine and science, thus predisposing them to more readily believe falsehoods regarding COVID-19. Social media has become a widely available source of both information and misinformation, such as on Twitter where rumors of COVID-19 spreading through 5G networks run rampant.15 16 In the absence of a strong science background and college-level courses, such rumors can gain traction and spread quickly. This puts the impetus on the scientific community to make statements regarding COVID-19 more readily accessible in terms of which platforms are used, and the language and presentation style.

The results of our study also demonstrated that risk perception about COVID-19 differed between countries, and the personal risk perception citizens felt was influenced by both country and income. Citizens in the UK were 1.5× as likely to perceive risk from COVID-19 compared with their US counterparts. As of January 2021, the USA has had 6842 cases of COVID-19 per 100,000 people, while the UK has had 4621 cases per 100,000 people.17 According to the HBM, the discrepancy of cases between these 2 countries may, at least in part, be a result of a decreased perceived risk of COVID-19 by American citizens.

Our results also established that lower income was associated with greater personal risk perception across all 3 countries. Compared with the lowest income quartile, the members of the highest income quartile were only 0.383× as likely to perceive a personal risk from COVID-19. This emphasizes the global impact of socioeconomic disparities on COVID-19. Studies have shown that lower income communities have been less able to socially distance. From the outset of the COVID-19 pandemic, wealthier areas decreased mobility significantly more than poorer areas. Wealthy areas went from the most mobile before the pandemic to the least mobile, while the poorest areas went from the least mobile to the most mobile. Additionally, research has shown lower income communities suffer more from pre-existing health conditions and reduced access to healthcare. These findings suggest a double burden of the COVID-19 pandemic on people in the lower income bracket.18

The correlation between personal risk and income is intensified within the USA. Compared with the lowest income bracket, the highest income bracket was only 0.205× as likely to perceive personal risk from COVID-19. This is consistent with the stark social inequities seen in US incomes. A study has shown that the death rate of COVID-19 per 100,000 person-years is 143.2 vs 83.3 in high-poverty versus low-poverty counties within the USA.19

Compared with the American citizens, Canadian citizens were only 0.403× as likely to perceive personal risk from COVID-19. Exploratory analysis has shown low levels of national preparedness, scale of testing and population characteristics were associated with increased national case load and overall mortality.20 This is consistent with the 6842 cases per 100,000 people of COVID-19 in the USA versus the 1793 cases of COVID-19 per 100,000 people of COVID-19 in Canada.17 Though there is not a significant difference between general risk perception of COVID-19 between the USA and Canada, Canadian citizens perceive less personal risk for the disease.

The increased personal risk perception seen in the USA, especially those in the lower income bracket, can potentially be due to the cost of healthcare. More than 78 million people in America do not have access to adequate health insurance.21 Health insurance in the USA is typically provided by employers, so as unemployment continues to rise, millions are at risk of losing their healthcare coverage. Moreover, US healthcare costs are relatively more expensive. For instance, more than 12 days in the intensive care unit on assisted ventilation in the USA could exceed US$80,000. Additionally, those who are underinsured are forced to pay debilitating out-of-pocket expenses which could be up to thousands of dollars in deductibles and copays.

Limitations

There are limitations to this study. First, the income quartiles used in the regression analysis did not take into account family size nor currency in the 3 countries. Additionally, demographic variables were dichotomized in order to answer our research question of whether risk perceptions and misperceptions were present between comparison groups. As a result, there was some loss of data. Possible confounding variables not included in the model could also present as a loss of data. Lastly, this study focuses on perceptions about COVID-19, and the findings may not be generalizable to perceptions about other health information.

Conclusion

We report on the first study to explore the roles of education and income in predicting misperceptions, risk perceptions, and personal risk perceptions about COVID-19 among residents in the USA, Canada, and UK. The present work indicates the statistically significant findings that education and income play a role in predicting misperceptions; country of origin predicts risk perception; and country of origin and income predict personal risk perception. Our findings support the importance of education and income in affecting health perceptions and outcomes. Further research is needed to explore interventions to minimize misperceptions, accurately shape risk perception, and effectively communicate science.

Data availability statement

Data are available in a public, open access repository. All data relevant to the study are included in the article or uploaded as supplementary information. Data used from this study were found at the following link: https://osf.io/3a497/

Ethics statements

Patient consent for publication

Not required.

Acknowledgments

We sincerely thank the healthcare professionals who risk their lives to help stop the spread of COVID-19, including Dr Tawfiqul Bhuiya. We also thank Dr Gordon Pennycook and the team for providing the data to inform this study. Lastly, we would like to thank Dr Doreen Olvet for her statistical oversight throughout the project.

Footnotes

  • Contributors All four authors contributed equally to the design of the work, analysis of the data, drafting of the work, final approval, and overall accuracy and integrity of the research.

  • Funding The authors have not declared a specific grant for this research from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

  • Competing interests None declared.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

  • Supplemental material This content has been supplied by the author(s). It has not been vetted by BMJ Publishing Group Limited (BMJ) and may not have been peer-reviewed. Any opinions or recommendations discussed are solely those of the author(s) and are not endorsed by BMJ. BMJ disclaims all liability and responsibility arising from any reliance placed on the content. Where the content includes any translated material, BMJ does not warrant the accuracy and reliability of the translations (including but not limited to local regulations, clinical guidelines, terminology, drug names and drug dosages), and is not responsible for any error and/or omissions arising from translation and adaptation or otherwise.

This article is made freely available for use in accordance with BMJ’s website terms and conditions for the duration of the covid-19 pandemic or until otherwise determined by BMJ. You may use, download and print the article for any lawful, non-commercial purpose (including text and data mining) provided that all copyright notices and trade marks are retained.

https://bmj.com/coronavirus/usage

References

  1. ↵
    Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) - events as they happen. Available: https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019/events-as-they-happen [Accessed 11 Jan 2021].
  2. ↵
    1. Zhu N ,
    2. Zhang D ,
    3. Wang W , et al
    . A novel coronavirus from patients with pneumonia in China, 2019. N Engl J Med 2020;382:727–33.doi:10.1056/NEJMoa2001017 pmid:http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31978945
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Shi Y ,
    2. Wang G ,
    3. Cai X-P , et al
    . An overview of COVID-19. J Zhejiang Univ Sci B 2020;21:343–60.doi:10.1631/jzus.B2000083 pmid:http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32425000
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Staff A
    . A Timeline of COVID-19 developments in 2020. Available: https://www.ajmc.com/view/a-timeline-of-covid19-developments-in-2020 [Accessed 11 Jan 2021].
  5. ↵
    1. The Toronto Star
    . A timeline of events in Canada’s fight against COVID-19, 2020. Available: https://www.thestar.com/news/canada/2020/06/18/a-timeline-of-events-in-canadas-fight-against-covid-19.html [Accessed 11 Jan 2021].
  6. ↵
    Lockdown timeline: what happened when the UK stayed at home? Available: https://www.expressandstar.com/news/uk-news/2020/07/02/lockdown-timeline-what-happened-when-the-uk-stayed-at-home/ [Accessed 11 Jan 2021].
  7. ↵
    1. Press Association Reporters
    . Coronavirus: Timeline of key events since UK was put into lockdown six months ago. The independent, 2020. Available: https://www.independent.co.uk/news/uk/home-news/coronavirus-uk-timeline-lockdown-boris-johnson-pubs-test-and-trace-vaccine-b547630.html [Accessed 11 Jan 2021].
  8. ↵
    1. Lewis T
    . Nine COVID-19 myths that just won’t go away, 2020. Scientific American. Available: https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/nine-covid-19-myths-that-just-wont-go-away/ [Accessed 11 Jan 2021].
  9. ↵
    1. Duan T ,
    2. Jiang H ,
    3. Deng X , et al
    . Government intervention, risk perception, and the adoption of protective action recommendations: evidence from the COVID-19 prevention and control experience of China. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2020;17. doi:doi:10.3390/ijerph17103387. [Epub ahead of print: 13 May 2020].pmid:http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32414013
  10. ↵
    1. Carpenter CJ
    . A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of health belief model variables in predicting behavior. Health Commun 2010;25:661–9.doi:10.1080/10410236.2010.521906 pmid:http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21153982
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  11. ↵
    1. Sulat JS ,
    2. Prabandari YS ,
    3. Sanusi R
    . The validity of health belief model variables in predicting behavioral change. Health Educ 2018 https://www.emerald.com/insight/content/doi/
  12. ↵
    1. Pennycook G ,
    2. McPhetres J ,
    3. Bago B
    . Attitudes about COVID-19 in Canada, the U.K., and the U.S.A.: a novel test of political polarization and motivated Reasoning. Pers Soc Psychol Bull.doi:10.31234/osf.io/zhjkp
  13. ↵
    1. Raifman MA ,
    2. Raifman JR
    . Disparities in the population at risk of severe illness from COVID-19 by Race/Ethnicity and income. Am J Prev Med 2020;59:137–9.doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2020.04.003 pmid:http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32430225
    OpenUrlPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. IBM CR
    . IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 27.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp, 2020.
  15. ↵
    1. Ahmed W ,
    2. Vidal-Alaball J ,
    3. Downing J , et al
    . COVID-19 and the 5G conspiracy theory: social network analysis of twitter data. J Med Internet Res 2020;22:e19458. doi:10.2196/19458 pmid:32352383
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Gupta L ,
    2. Gasparyan AY ,
    3. Misra DP , et al
    . Information and misinformation on COVID-19: a cross-sectional survey study. J Korean Med Sci 2020;35:e256. doi:10.3346/jkms.2020.35.e256 pmid:http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32657090
    OpenUrlPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. CNN
    . Tracking coronavirus’ global spread. Available: https://www.cnn.com/interactive/2020/health/coronavirus-maps-and-cases [Accessed 11 Jan 2021].
  18. ↵
    1. Weill JA ,
    2. Stigler M ,
    3. Deschenes O , et al
    . Social distancing responses to COVID-19 emergency declarations strongly differentiated by income. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2020;117:19658–60.doi:10.1073/pnas.2009412117 pmid:http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32727905
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Chen JT ,
    2. Krieger N
    . Revealing the unequal burden of COVID-19 by income, race/ethnicity, and household crowding: US county versus ZIP code analyses. COVID-19 and public health: looking back, moing forward. J Public Health Manag Pract 2021;27:S43–56.doi:10.1097/PHH.0000000000001263
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Chaudhry R ,
    2. Dranitsaris G ,
    3. Mubashir T , et al
    . A country level analysis measuring the impact of government actions, country preparedness and socioeconomic factors on COVID-19 mortality and related health outcomes. EClinicalMedicine 2020;25:100464.doi:10.1016/j.eclinm.2020.100464 pmid:http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32838237
    OpenUrlPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Galvani AP ,
    2. Parpia AS ,
    3. Foster EM , et al
    . Improving the prognosis of health care in the USA. Lancet 2020;395:524–33.doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(19)33019-3 pmid:http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32061298
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top
Vol 69 Issue 8 Table of Contents
Journal of Investigative Medicine: 69 (8)
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • AFMR Highlights
  • Front Matter (PDF)
Email

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on JIM.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Predictors of misperceptions, risk perceptions, and personal risk perceptions about COVID-19 by country, education and income
(Your Name) has sent you a message from JIM
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the JIM web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Print
Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Citation Tools
Predictors of misperceptions, risk perceptions, and personal risk perceptions about COVID-19 by country, education and income
Tanzim Bhuiya, Richard Klares III, Madellena A Conte, Joseph Steven Cervia
Journal of Investigative Medicine Dec 2021, 69 (8) 1473-1478; DOI: 10.1136/jim-2021-001835

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Cite This
  • APA
  • Chicago
  • Endnote
  • MLA
Loading
Predictors of misperceptions, risk perceptions, and personal risk perceptions about COVID-19 by country, education and income
Tanzim Bhuiya, Richard Klares III, Madellena A Conte, Joseph Steven Cervia
Journal of Investigative Medicine Dec 2021, 69 (8) 1473-1478; DOI: 10.1136/jim-2021-001835
Download PDF

Share
Predictors of misperceptions, risk perceptions, and personal risk perceptions about COVID-19 by country, education and income
Tanzim Bhuiya, Richard Klares III, Madellena A Conte, Joseph Steven Cervia
Journal of Investigative Medicine Dec 2021, 69 (8) 1473-1478; DOI: 10.1136/jim-2021-001835
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Respond to this article
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One
  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Conclusion
    • Data availability statement
    • Ethics statements
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • eLetters
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Organochlorine pesticides, oxidative stress biomarkers, and leukemia: a case-control study
  • Temporal trends and disparities in gastroenterology care use before, during, and after COVID-19 lockdown
  • Opium may affect coronary artery disease by inducing inflammation but not through the expression of CD9, CD36, and CD68
Show more Original research

Similar Articles

 

CONTENT

  • Latest content
  • Current issue
  • Archive
  • Sign up for email alerts
  • RSS

JOURNAL

  • About the journal
  • Editorial board
  • Subscribe
  • Thank you to our reviewers
  • American Federation for Medical Research

AUTHORS

  • Information for authors
  • Submit a paper
  • Track your article
  • Open Access at BMJ

HELP

  • Contact us
  • Reprints
  • Permissions
  • Advertising
  • Feedback form

© 2023 American Federation for Medical Research